Orthophony
Improving communication in deficients
This year twenty-five people who have been unable to talk
since birth will take part in research aimed at making
their communication with the outside world easier. The
two researchers responsible for this work, Natacha Trudeau
and Ann Sutton, are convinced that they can provide persons
who are nearly mute with better tools.
Some 200,000 Canadians suffer from speech deficiency,
which is sometimes acquired, but most often present from
birth. While not completely mute—since they can produce
sounds and even pronounce a few words—they obviously cannot
use speech to make themselves understood in everyday life.
However, their hearing is intact. Frequently, their crippling
inability to communicate is accompanied by a serious motor
disorder.
In earlier research, Ms Sutton was able to show that persons
with this deficiency apparently did not have the same syntactic
flexibility as people with full speech. She designed a
test in which participants used ideograms to reproduce
simple sentences that nevertheless required the speakers
to adopt a strategy to make themselves understood. The
sentence was the following: “The girl pushing the clown
is wearing a hat.” Small Playmobil characters and accessories
were reproduced (on computer or on cardboard), including
a girl, a baby carriage, a hat, and a clown. People with
speech (25 in all) changed the order of the phrase to “Girl
hat carriage clown,” which gave “The girl wearing a hat
pushes the clown.” They therefore used the strategy of
word proximity. The others did not invert the initial order
of words, and left the hat at the end of the sentence,
which requires the word “girl” to be repeated. This gave “The
girl pushes the clown. The girl wears a hat.”
Some participants had serious motor difficulties and took
up to 45 seconds to produce a single image. Some of them
were unable to use their arms or legs, or did not have
use of either. But the principle of least effort hardly
means that it is impossible to improve training of the
participants. Perhaps we have forgotten to teach them certain
skills. But which skills?
When thinking about the importance of the representation
of language in the ability to communicate other than by
words, Natacha Trudeau and Ann Sutton have not excluded
the hypothesis that persons who are unable to speak do
not receive adequate education, in particular due to preconceived
ideas or prejudices about their ability to learn. “When
we have difficulty understanding a child, we over- or under-estimate
his skills. But the more correct our vision, the better
things will go,” Ann Sutton remarks. “And if this child
can learn to read and write, this will open doors for him.”
|